Skip to content
Snippets Groups Projects
Commit 5701c9f2 authored by Rachel Heyard's avatar Rachel Heyard
Browse files

polishing intro (far from done/happy)

parent 68fffbee
No related branches found
No related tags found
No related merge requests found
......@@ -96,6 +96,20 @@
journal = {BMJ}
}
@article{Goodman2008,
doi = {10.1053/j.seminhematol.2008.04.003},
url = {https://doi.org/10.1053/j.seminhematol.2008.04.003},
year = {2008},
month = jul,
publisher = {Elsevier {BV}},
volume = {45},
number = {3},
pages = {135--140},
author = {Steven Goodman},
title = {A Dirty Dozen: Twelve P-Value Misconceptions},
journal = {Seminars in Hematology}
}
@Article{Bayarri2003,
doi = {10.1016/s0378-3758(02)00282-3},
year = {2003},
......@@ -830,6 +844,15 @@ url = {www.fda.gov/regulatory-information/search-fda-guidance-documents/providi
title = {New preprint server for medical research},
journal = {{BMJ}}
}
@book{NSF2019,
doi = {10.17226/25303},
url = {https://doi.org/10.17226/25303},
year = {2019},
month = sep,
publisher = {National Academies Press},
author = {{National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine}},
title = {Reproducibility and Replicability in Science}
}
@Manual{Gehlenborg2019,
title = {UpSetR: A More Scalable Alternative to Venn and Euler Diagrams for
......
......@@ -24,7 +24,7 @@
bottom=25mm,
}
\title{\bf Meta-research: Replication studies and absence of evidence}
\title{\bf Meta-research: Replication studies and the ``absence of evidence''}
\author{{\bf Rachel Heyard, Charlotte Micheloud, Samuel Pawel, Leonhard Held} \\
Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Prevention Institute \\
Center for Reproducible Science \\
......@@ -122,9 +122,9 @@ formatBF <- Vectorize(FUN = formatBF.)
replicating or even proving a null-effect. Methods to adequately summarize
the evidence for the null have been proposed. With this paper we want to
highlight the consequences of the ``absence of evidence'' fallacy in the
replication setting and want to guide the readers and hopefully future
authors of replication studies to the correct methods to design and
analyse their replication attempts.
replication setting and want to guide the reader and future
author of replication studies to existing methods to appropriately
design and analyse replication attempts of non-significant findings.
} \\
\rule{\textwidth}{0.5pt} \emph{Keywords}: Bayesian hypothesis testing,
equivalence test, non-inferiority test, null hypothesis, replication
......@@ -142,25 +142,41 @@ participants, n) is used to achieve an 80-90\% power of correctly rejecting the
null hypothesis. This leaves us with a 10-20\% chance of a false negative.
Somehow this fact from ``Hypothesis Testing 101'' is all too often forgotten and
studies showing an effect with a p-value larger than the conventionally used
significance level of $\alpha = 0.05$ is doomed to be a ``negative study'' or showing a
significance level of $\alpha = 0.05$ are doomed to be a ``negative study'' or showing a
``null effect''. Some have even called to abolish the term ``negative
study'' altogether, as every well-designed and conducted study is a ``positive
study'' altogether, as every well-designed and well-conducted study is a ``positive
contribution to knowledge'', regardless it’s results \citep{Chalmers1002}. Others
suggest to shift away from significance testing because of the many misconceptions
of $p$-values and significance \citep{Berner2022}.
More specifically, turning to the replication context, ``the absence of evidence'' fallacy
appeared in the definitions of replication success in some of the large-scale
replication projects. The Replication Project Cancer Biology \citep[RPCB]{Errington2021}
and the RP in Experimental Philosophy \citep[RPEP]{Cova2018} explicitly define a
replication of a non-significant original effect as successful if the effect in the
replication study is also non-significant. While the authors of the RPEP warn
the reader that the use of p-values as criterion for success is problematic when
applied to replications of original non-significant findings, the authors of the
RPCB do not. The RP in Psychological Science \citep{Opensc2015}, on the other hand,
excluded the ``original nulls'' when deciding replication success based on significance and
the Social Science RP \citep{Camerer2018} as well as the RP in Experimental Economics
\cite{Camerer2016} did not include original studies without a significant finding.
of $p$-values and significance \citep{Goodman2008, Berner2022}.
Turning to the replication context, replicability has been
defined as ``obtaining consistent results across studies aimed at answering the
same scientific question, each of which has obtained its own data'' \citep{NSF2019}.
Hence, a replication of an original finding attempts to find consistent results while
applying the same methods and protocol as published in the original study on newly collected data.
In the past decade, big collaborations of researcher and research groups conducted
large-scale replication projects (RP) to estimate the replicability of their respective research
field. In these projects, a set of high impact and influential original studies were
selected to be replicated as close as possible to the original methodology. The
results and conclusions of the RPs showed alarmingly low levels of replicability in most fields.
The Replication Project Cancer Biology \citep[RPCB]{Errington2021}, the RP in
Experimental Philosophy \citep[RPEP]{Cova2018} and the RP in Psychological
Science \citep[RPP]{Opensc2015} also attempted to replicate original studies with
non-significant effects. The authors of those RPs unfortunately fell into the
``absence of evidence''-fallacy trap when defining successful replications.
More specifically, the RPCB and RPEP explicitly define a replication of a non-significant
original effect as successful if the effect in the replication study is also non-significant.
While the authors of the RPEP warn the reader that the use of $p$-values as criterion
for success is problematic when applied to replications of original non-significant findings,
the authors of the RPCB do not. In the RPP, on the other hand, ``original nulls''
were excluded when assessing replication success based on significance.
% In general, using the significance criterion as definition of replication success
% arises from a false interpretation of the failure to find evidence against the null
% hypothesis as evidence for the null. Non-significant original finding does not
% mean that the underlying true effect is zero nor that it does not exist. This is
% especially true if the original study is under-powered.
\textbf{To replicate or not to replicate an original ``null'' finding?}
Because of the previously presented fallacy, original studies with
......@@ -174,41 +190,28 @@ successful replication we need a ``significant result in the same direction in
both the original and the replication study'' (i.e. the two-trials rule, \cite{Senn2008}),
replicating a non-significant original result does indeed not make any sense.
However, the use of significance as sole criterion for replication success has
its shortcomings.
\citet{Anderson2016} summarized the goals of replications and recommended analyses and
success criterion. Interestingly they recommended using the two-trials rule only if
the goal is to infer the \textit{existence and direction} of a statistical significant
effect, while the replicating researchers are not interested in the size of this effect.
A successful replication attempt would result in a small $p$-value, while a large $p$-value
in the replication would only mean that the
On the contrary, if the goal is to infer a null effect \cite{Anderson2016} write that,
in this case, evidence for the null hypothesis has to be provided. To achieve this
goal equivalence tests or Bayesian methods to quantify the evidence for the null
hypothesis can be used. In the following, we will illustrate how to accurately
interpret the potential replication of original non-significant results in the
Cancer Biology Replication Project.
% \todo[inline]{SP: look and discuss the papers from \citet{Anderson2016, Anderson2017}}
\todo[inline]{RH: Note sure what to cite from \citet{Anderson2017}}
In general a non-significant original finding does not mean that the underlying
true effect is zero nor that it does not exist. This is especially true if the
original study is under-powered. \todo[inline]{RH: for myself, more blabla on
under-powered original studies}
its shortcomings and other definitions for replication success have been proposed
\cite{Simonsohn2015, Ly2018, Hedges2019, Held2020}. Additionally, replication
studies have to be well-design too in order to ensure high enough replication power
\cite{Anderson2017, Micheloud2020}.
According to \citet{Anderson2016}, if the goal of a replications is to infer a null effect
evidence for the null hypothesis has to be provided. To achieve this they recommend to use
equivalence tests or Bayesian methods to quantify the evidence for the null hypothesis can be used.
In the following, we will illustrate how to accurately interpret the potential
replication of original non-significant results in the Replication Project Cancer Biology.
\section{Example: ``Null findings'' from the Replication Project Cancer
Biology}
Of the 158 effects presented in 23 original studies that were repeated in the
cancer biology RP \citep{Errington2021} 14\% (22) were interpreted as ``null
RPCB \citep{Errington2021} 14\% (22) were interpreted as ``null
effects''.
% One of those repeated effects with a non-significant original finding was
% presented in Lu et al. (2014) and replicated by Richarson et al (2016).
Note that the attempt to replicate all the experiments from the original study
was not completed because of some unforeseen issues in the implementation (see
\cite{Errington2021b} for more details on the unfinished registered reports in
the RPCB). Figure~\ref{fig:nullfindings} shows effect estimates with confidence
intervals for the original ``null findings'' (with $p_{o} > 0.05$) and their
% Note that the attempt to replicate all the experiments from the original study
% was not completed because of some unforeseen issues in the implementation (see
% \cite{Errington2021b} for more details on the unfinished registered reports in
% the RPCB).
Figure~\ref{fig:nullfindings} shows effect estimates with confidence
intervals for these original ``null findings'' (with $p_{o} > 0.05$) and their
replication studies from the project.
% The replication of our example effect (Paper \# 47, Experiment \# 1, Effect \#
% 5) was however completed. The authors of the original study declared that
......@@ -223,16 +226,6 @@ replication studies from the project.
% effect sizes together with their 95\% confidence intervals and respective
% two-sided p-values.
\todo[inline]{SP: I have used the original $p$-values as reported in the data
set to select the studies in the figure . I think in this way we have the data
correctly identified as the RPCP paper reports that there are 20 null findings
in the ``All outcomes'' category. I wonder how they go from the all outcomes
category to the ``effects'' category (15 null findings), perhaps pool the
internal replications by meta-analysis? I think it would be better to stay in
the all outcomes category, but of course it needs to be discussed. Also some
of the $p$-values were probably computed in a different way than under
normality (e.g., the $p$-value from (47, 1, 6, 1) under normality is clearly
significant).}
<< "data" >>=
## data
......@@ -282,53 +275,31 @@ rpcbNull <- rpcb %>%
@
\begin{figure}[!htb]
<< "plot-p-values", fig.height = 3.5 >>=
## check discrepancy between reported and recomputed p-values for null results
pbreaks <- c(0.005, 0.02, 0.05, 0.15, 0.4)
ggplot(data = rpcbNull, aes(x = po, y = po2)) +
geom_abline(intercept = 0, slope = 1, alpha = 0.2) +
geom_vline(xintercept = 0.05, alpha = 0.2, lty = 2) +
geom_hline(yintercept = 0.05, alpha = 0.2, lty = 2) +
geom_point(alpha = 0.8, shape = 21, fill = "darkgrey") +
geom_label_repel(data = filter(rpcbNull, po2 < 0.05),
aes(x = po, y = po2, label = id), alpha = 0.8, size = 3,
min.segment.length = 0, box.padding = 0.7) +
labs(x = bquote(italic(p["o"]) ~ "(reported)"),
y = bquote(italic(p["o"]) ~ "(recomputed under normality)")) +
scale_x_log10(breaks = pbreaks, label = scales::percent) +
scale_y_log10(breaks = pbreaks, labels = scales::percent) +
coord_fixed(xlim = c(min(c(rpcbNull$po2, rpcbNull$po)), 1),
ylim = c(min(c(rpcbNull$po2, rpcbNull$po)), 1)) +
theme_bw() +
theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank())
@
\caption{Reported versus recomputed under normality two-sided $p$-values from
original studies declared as ``null findings'' ($p_{o} > 0.05$) in
Reproducibility Project: Cancer Biology \citep{Errington2021}.}
\end{figure}
\begin{figure}[!htb]
<< "plot-null-findings-rpcb", fig.height = 8.5 >>=
<< "plot-null-findings-rpcb", fig.height =8.5 >>=
ggplot(data = rpcbNull) +
facet_wrap(~ id, scales = "free", ncol = 4) +
geom_hline(yintercept = 0, lty = 2, alpha = 0.5) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Original", y = smdo, ymin = smdo - 2*so,
ymax = smdo + 2*so)) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Replication", y = smdr, ymin = smdr - 2*sr,
ymax = smdr + 2*sr)) +
geom_text(aes(x = "Replication", y = pmax(smdr + 2.1*sr, smdo + 2.1*so),
label = paste("'BF'['01']",
ifelse(BFrformat == "< 1/1000", "", "=="),
BFrformat)),
parse = TRUE, size = 3,
nudge_y = -0.5) +
labs(x = "", y = "Standardized mean difference (SMD)") +
theme_bw() +
theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank(),
panel.grid.major.x = element_blank())
facet_wrap(~ id, scales = "free", ncol = 4) +
geom_hline(yintercept = 0, lty = 2, alpha = 0.5) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Original", y = smdo, ymin = smdo - 2*so,
ymax = smdo + 2*so)) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Replication", y = smdr, ymin = smdr - 2*sr,
ymax = smdr + 2*sr)) +
labs(x = "", y = "Standardized mean difference (SMD)") +
geom_text(aes(x = 1.4, y = smdo, #pmin(smdr - 2.2*sr, smdo - 2.2*so),
label = paste("n[o]==", no)), col = "darkblue",
parse = TRUE, size = 2.5,
nudge_x = -.05) +
geom_text(aes(x = 2.4, y = smdr, #pmin(smdr - 2.2*sr, smdo - 2.2*so),
label = paste("n[r]==", nr)), col = "darkblue",
parse = TRUE, size = 2.5,
nudge_x = -.05) +
theme_bw() +
theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank(),
panel.grid.major.x = element_blank())
# TODO: one replication is missing, id == "(37, 2, 2, 1)"
# what should we do with it?
@
\caption{Standardized mean difference effect estimates with 95\% confidence
......@@ -338,12 +309,14 @@ ggplot(data = rpcbNull) +
number, Effect number, Internal replication number). The data were downloaded
from \url{https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/e5nvr}. The relevant variables were
extracted from the file ``\texttt{RP\_CB Final Analysis - Effect level
data.csv}''.}
data.csv}''. Additionally the original ($n_o$) and replication sample sizes
($n_r$) are indicated in each plot.}
\label{fig:nullfindings}
\end{figure}
\section{Dealing with original non-significant findings in replication projects}
\subsection{Equivalence Design}
For many years, equivalence designs have been used in clinical trials to
understand whether a new drug, which might be cheaper or have less side effects
......@@ -384,10 +357,85 @@ absence of evidence for either hypothesis ($\BF_{01} \approx 1$).
% the replication Bayes factor \citep{Verhagen2014}.
\begin{figure}[!htb]
<< "plot-null-findings-rpcb-br", fig.height = 8.5 >>=
ggplot(data = rpcbNull) +
facet_wrap(~ id, scales = "free", ncol = 4) +
geom_hline(yintercept = 0, lty = 2, alpha = 0.5) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Original", y = smdo, ymin = smdo - 2*so,
ymax = smdo + 2*so)) +
geom_pointrange(aes(x = "Replication", y = smdr, ymin = smdr - 2*sr,
ymax = smdr + 2*sr)) +
geom_text(aes(x = "Replication", y = pmax(smdr + 2.1*sr, smdo + 2.1*so),
label = paste("'BF'['01']",
ifelse(BFrformat == "< 1/1000", "", "=="),
BFrformat)),
parse = TRUE, size = 3,
nudge_y = -0.5) +
labs(x = "", y = "Standardized mean difference (SMD)") +
theme_bw() +
theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank(),
panel.grid.major.x = element_blank())
@
\caption{Standardized mean difference effect estimates with 95\% confidence
interval for the ``null findings'' (with $p_{o} > 0.05$) and their replication
studies from the Reproducibility Project: Cancer Biology \citep{Errington2021}.
The identifier above each plot indicates (Original paper number, Experiment
number, Effect number, Internal replication number). The data were downloaded
from \url{https://doi.org/10.17605/osf.io/e5nvr}. The relevant variables were
extracted from the file ``\texttt{RP\_CB Final Analysis - Effect level
data.csv}''.}
\label{fig:nullfindings}
\end{figure}
\bibliographystyle{apalikedoiurl}
\bibliography{bibliography}
\appendix
\section{Note on $p$-values}
\todo[inline]{SP: I have used the original $p$-values as reported in the data
set to select the studies in the figure . I think in this way we have the data
correctly identified as the RPCP paper reports that there are 20 null findings
in the ``All outcomes'' category. I wonder how they go from the all outcomes
category to the ``effects'' category (15 null findings), perhaps pool the
internal replications by meta-analysis? I think it would be better to stay in
the all outcomes category, but of course it needs to be discussed. Also some
of the $p$-values were probably computed in a different way than under
normality (e.g., the $p$-value from (47, 1, 6, 1) under normality is clearly
significant).}
\begin{figure}[!htb]
<< "plot-p-values", fig.height = 3.5 >>=
## check discrepancy between reported and recomputed p-values for null results
pbreaks <- c(0.005, 0.02, 0.05, 0.15, 0.4)
ggplot(data = rpcbNull, aes(x = po, y = po2)) +
geom_abline(intercept = 0, slope = 1, alpha = 0.2) +
geom_vline(xintercept = 0.05, alpha = 0.2, lty = 2) +
geom_hline(yintercept = 0.05, alpha = 0.2, lty = 2) +
geom_point(alpha = 0.8, shape = 21, fill = "darkgrey") +
geom_label_repel(data = filter(rpcbNull, po2 < 0.05),
aes(x = po, y = po2, label = id), alpha = 0.8, size = 3,
min.segment.length = 0, box.padding = 0.7) +
labs(x = bquote(italic(p["o"]) ~ "(reported)"),
y = bquote(italic(p["o"]) ~ "(recomputed under normality)")) +
scale_x_log10(breaks = pbreaks, label = scales::percent) +
scale_y_log10(breaks = pbreaks, labels = scales::percent) +
coord_fixed(xlim = c(min(c(rpcbNull$po2, rpcbNull$po)), 1),
ylim = c(min(c(rpcbNull$po2, rpcbNull$po)), 1)) +
theme_bw() +
theme(panel.grid.minor = element_blank())
@
\caption{Reported versus recomputed under normality two-sided $p$-values from
original studies declared as ``null findings'' ($p_{o} > 0.05$) in
Reproducibility Project: Cancer Biology \citep{Errington2021}.}
\end{figure}
<< "sessionInfo1", eval = Reproducibility, results = "asis" >>=
## print R sessionInfo to see system information and package versions
......
0% Loading or .
You are about to add 0 people to the discussion. Proceed with caution.
Finish editing this message first!
Please register or to comment